Modelo de desbalance del desarrollo cerebral: nuevo enfoque teórico en la comprensión de conductas de riesgo en la adolescencia

Yunier Broche Pérez

Texto completo:

PDF (314 KB)

Resumen

OBJETIVO: Describir el modelo de desbalance del desarrollo cerebral y presentar la evidencia empírica que sustenta esta hipótesis sobre el neurodesarrollo en la adolescencia.

DESARROLLO: La adolescencia representa un periodo crítico en la maduración cerebral con importantes implicaciones en las dimensiones afectivo–sociales y cognitiva. En esta etapa se aprecia un incremento en las conductas de riesgo y las dificultades en la toma de decisiones. Las teorías clásicas explican esta peculiaridad aludiendo a la inmadurez de un grupo de procesos cognitivos superiores, sin embargo, la evidencia reciente indica que las habilidades básicas de procesamiento de la información y sus substratos neurales han alcanzado su punto máximo de desarrollo en este periodo. El modelo de desbalance del desarrollo cerebral es un enfoque teórico novedoso, con base en las neurociencias, que ofrece una explicación alternativa sobre la prevalencia de conductas de riesgo y los déficits tradicionales en la toma de decisiones durante la adolescencia. El modelo plantea la existencia de un desarrollo asincrónico entre las estructuras prefrontales en relación con las áreas neurales relacionadas con los procesos socioemocionales. Particularmente, las conexiones subcorticales motivacionales y emocionales se desarrollan más temprano de lo que lo hacen las regiones relacionadas al control prefrontal. Este desequilibrio en la maduración conlleva en un aumento de la dominancia de las regiones motivacionales subcorticales en comparación con las regiones prefrontales.

CONCLUSIONES: El desbalance en la maduración de la CPF impide el desarrollo óptimo del sistema de control cognitivo (SCC), permitiendo que el sistema socioemocional (SSE) ejerza un rol rector en el control del comportamiento en esta edad.

Palabras clave

Adolescencia; Conductas de riesgo; Desarrollo cerebral; Modelo de desbalance del desarrollo cerebral; Neurociencias

Referencias

Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2006;47:296–312.

Giedd JN. Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2014;10(21):77–85.

Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–63.

Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:947–56.

Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcom SE, Henkenius A, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:309–15.

Broche–Pérez Y, Cruz–López D. Toma de decisiones en la adolescencia: Entre la razón y la emoción. Ciencia Cognitiva 2014;8(3):70–2.

Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Pubertal development and behaviour: Hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies. Brain Cogn. 2010 Feb;72(1):66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.007.

Sisk CL, Zehr JL. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behaviour. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2005;26:163–74.

Wigfield A, Byrnes BJ, Eccles JS. Adolescent development. En: Alexander PA, Winne W, (eds). Handbook of Educational Psychology. Erlbaum: Mahwah; 2006; p.87–113.

Romeo RD. The teenage brain: The stress response and the adolescent brain. Curr Dir Psychol. Sci.2013;22:140–5.

Spear L. The teenage brain: Adolescents and alcohol. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22:152–7.

Dahl RE. Affect regulation, brain development, and behavioral/emotional health in adolescence. CNS Spectrums. 2011;6:60–72.

Casey BJ, Caudle K. The Teenage Brain: Self Control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22(2):82–7.

Somerville LH, Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:236–41.

Geier CF, Terwilliger R, Teslovich T, Velanova K, Luna B. Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1613–29.

Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1–10.

Cohen JR, Asarnow RF, Sabb FW, Bilder RM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ, et al. A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:669–71.

Van–Leijenhorst L, Zanolie K, Van Meel CS, Westenberg PM, Rombouts SA, Crone EA. What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:61–9.

Bell CC, McBride DF. Affect regulation and prevention of risky behaviors. JAMA. 2010;304:565–66.

Piaget J, Inhelder B. The origin o fthe idea of chance in children. Norton: Oxford; 1975.

DeLoache JS, Miller KF, Pierroutsakos SL. Reasoning and problem solving. En: Kuhn D, Siegler RS, (eds). Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language. Wiley: NewYork; 1998. p. 826–50.

Siegler RS. Emerging minds: The process of change inchildren’s thinking. NewYork: Oxford University Press; 1996.

Kuhn D, García–Milá M, Zohar A, Andersen C. Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1995;60(4):1–128.

Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A. The adolescent brain. Dev Rev. 2011;28:62–77.

Galvan A, Hare TA, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ. Risk–taking and the adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Dev Sci. 2007;10:8–14.

Casey BJ, Galvan A, Hare TA. Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15:239–44.

Keating D. Cognitive and brain development. En: Lerner R, Steinberg L, (eds).Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. New York: Wiley; 2014. p. 45–84.

Figner B, Mackinlay JR, Wilkening F, Weber UE. Affective Deliberative Processes in Risky Choice: Age Differences in Risk Taking in the Columbia Card Task. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009;35:709–30.

Steinberg LA. Social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28:78–106.

Cohen JD. The vulcanization of the human brain: A neural perspective on interactions between cognition and emotion. J. Econ. Perspect. 2005; 19: 3–24.

Leijenhorst LV, Moor BG, Macks ZA, Serge ARB, Westenberg PM, Crone EA. Adolescent risky decision–making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage. 2010;51:345–55.

Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk–taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6885–92.

Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. CDPS. 2007;16:55–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–8721.2007.00475.x

Steinberg L. Adolescent development and juvenile justice. Annu Rev Cli Psychol. 2010;5:459–85.

Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 2010;72:124–33.

Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW. Incentive–elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults J Neurosci. 2004 Feb 25;24(8):1793–802.

Ernst M, Nelson EE, Jazbe S, McClure EB, Monk CS, Leibenluft E, Pine DS. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2005;25:1279–91.

Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M. Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: Development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologica. 2006;45:1270–79.

Paus T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:60–8.

Leenrot R, Gotagy N, Greenstein D, Wells E, Wallace G, Clasen L. Sexual dimorphism of brain development trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage. 2007;12:20–7.

Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM. Personality and Risk–Taking: Common Bisocial Factors. J Pers. 2010;68:999–1029.

Chambers R, Taylor J, Potenza M. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;160:1041–52.

Spear LP. The adolescent brain and the college drinker: biological basis of the propensity to use and misuse alcohol. J Stud Alcohol. 2012;14:17–81.

Millstein SG, Halpern–Felsher BL. Perceptions of risk and vulnerability. J Adolesc Health. 2012;31:10–27.

Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk–taking, risk preference, and risky decision–making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Dev Psychol. 2005;41:625–35.

Van–Duijvenvoorde ACK, OpdeMacks ZA, Overgaauwa S, Moor BG, Dahl RE, Crone EA. A cross–sectional and longitudinal analysis of reward–related brain activation: Effects of age, pubertal stage, and reward sensitivity. Brain Cogn. 2014 Aug;89:3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.10.005.

Peters S, Cédric P, Koolschijn MP, Crone EA, Van–Duijvenvoorde ACK, Raijmakers MEJ, Dahl RE. Strategies influence neural activity for feedback learning across child and adolescent development. Neuropsychologia. 2014;62:365–74.





Licencia de Creative Commons
Esta obra está bajo una licencia de Creative Commons Reconocimiento-NoComercial 4.0 Internacional.